违心的话是什么意思| 射手座和什么座最配| 6月13日是什么日子| 排卵期一般是什么时候| 平片是什么| hoegaarden是什么啤酒| 冉是什么意思| 脚底长水泡是什么原因| 药物流产后吃什么好| 7.14号是什么节日| 护士证什么时候下来| 京东发什么快递| 又什么又什么的葡萄| 下半夜咳嗽是什么原因| 拆穿是什么意思| 叶黄素是什么| 教师编制是什么意思| 腿肿吃什么药| 脾虚湿蕴证是什么意思| 幼小衔接是什么意思| 柠檬黄配什么颜色好看| 为什么吃芒果会过敏| 为什么会肌酐高| 炒什么菜好吃又简单| 小儿消化不良吃什么药最好| 急性扁桃体化脓是什么原因引起的| 巴结是什么意思| 耳朵里痒是什么原因| 冷藏是什么意思| 早上7点是什么时辰| 沉的右边念什么| 杏仁有什么功效和作用| 妨父母痣是什么意思| 非萎缩性胃炎吃什么药效果好| 蜈蚣代表什么生肖| 什么情况属于骗婚| 吃什么长个子| 监督是什么意思| c7是什么意思| 祭奠用什么花| 脾虚的人有什么症状| 玛瑙五行属什么| 什么脸型适合什么发型| 知了为什么要叫| 淋巴细胞比率偏高是什么意思| 感冒为什么会头痛| 眼轴是什么意思| 人体缺少蛋白质会有什么症状| 妥瑞氏症是什么病| 做ct挂什么科| 阳痿早泄吃什么药最好| 空腔是什么意思| 简历照片用什么底色| 勤字五行属什么| 孕激素是什么意思| 甘油三酯高是指什么| 包皮是什么意思| 强项是什么意思| 口舌是非是什么意思| 清真什么意思| 硬气是什么意思| 剥皮实草是什么意思| 双侧瞳孔缩小见于什么| 女生下面长什么样| 泉肌症是什么病| 非户籍是什么意思| 尿痛什么原因| 竹节麻是什么面料| 九重紫纪咏结局是什么| 梦见摘瓜是什么意思啊| 茜是什么意思| 键盘侠是什么意思| 六月初二是什么星座| 枸橼酸西地那非片有什么副作用| kg是什么意思| o3是什么| 拔牙第二天可以吃什么| 老鼠最怕什么东西| pvc是什么意思| 羡慕是什么意思| 大便干燥拉不出来是什么原因| 乘风破浪的意思是什么| 白癜风是什么病| 三级残疾是什么程度| 尿道下裂是什么意思| 郭靖属什么生肖| 福祉是什么意思| 讹诈是什么意思| 人为什么有五根手指| 为什么空调外机会滴水| dr和ct有什么区别| 梦见洗手是什么意思| 沙土地适合种什么农作物| 梦见吃杨梅是什么意思| 肝有问题会出现什么症状| 文竹的寓意是什么| 什么渐渐什么| 尘肺病用什么药最好| 静脉曲张吃什么药| 手指缝里长水泡还痒是什么原因| gt是什么| 小便有点黄是什么原因| 副团级是什么军衔| k9什么意思| 大姨妈来了可以吃什么水果| 做梦梦到蛆是什么意思| 早孕有什么反应| 膝关节疼痛挂什么科| 家财万贯是什么动物| 孕妇为什么那么怕热| ooxx是什么意思| 无情是什么意思| 后背疼是什么原因引起的| 什么血型的人最聪明| bun是什么意思| 阴虚火旺吃什么好| mA是什么| 红糖的原料是什么| 低血糖有什么症状| 微信中抱拳是什么意思| 晴雨伞是什么意思| 脉动是什么意思| 175是什么码| vam是什么意思| 小孩肚子疼吃什么药| 烟卡是什么| 癫痫是什么意思| 鼻窦炎是什么病| 乳腺术后吃什么最好| 脸肿眼睛肿是什么原因引起的| 大饼是什么意思| 万病之源是什么| 色觉异常是什么意思| 3月22日什么星座| 嗜碱性粒细胞偏低说明什么| 什么克水| 基础油是什么油| 恃势之刑是什么意思| 10月30是什么星座| 嘴巴周围长痘痘是什么原因| 清炖排骨放什么调料| 手脚出汗什么原因| 短效避孕药是什么| miss什么意思| 肾盂是什么意思| 市人大副主任什么级别| 诱发电位是检查什么病的| 明信片是什么| 厅局级是什么级别| 什么病会引起牙疼| 夫人是什么生肖| 纯天然无公害什么意思| 乳腺癌的症状是什么| 雷诺综合症是什么病| 夫妻肺片是什么| 大暑是什么时候| 为什么的拼音| 主动脉瓣退行性变是什么意思| 减肥中午吃什么| 锌过量会引发什么症状| 什么什么相接| 女人喜欢什么类型男人| 化疗期间吃什么食物好| l1是什么意思| 石女是什么意思啊| thx是什么意思| 4月21日什么星座| 肚脐两边疼是什么原因| 皮赘是什么| 螨虫用什么药膏| md是什么职位| 蒲公英和玫瑰花一起泡有什么功效| gms是什么意思| lhrh是什么激素| 尿微量白蛋白高是什么原因| 盐酸利多卡因是什么药| 健脾胃吃什么食物好| 肌腱属于什么组织| 木薯粉在超市里叫什么| 柳州有什么大学| 对立面是什么意思| 朋友妻不可欺是什么意思| 吃黑米有什么好处和坏处| 打一个喷嚏代表什么| 黄体破裂吃什么药| 诸葛亮儿子叫什么| 植物纤维是什么面料| 隐忍是什么意思| 眼睛发痒是什么原因| 又字五行属什么| 当归有什么作用和功效| 五月十二号是什么日子| 打嗝什么原因| 咖啡加牛奶叫什么| 12月10日什么星座| 通五行属什么| 胸部爱出汗是什么原因| 记过处分有什么影响| 珍珠婚是什么意思| 子宫癌有什么症状| 为什么嗓子总有痰| 优柔寡断是什么意思| 下边瘙痒是什么原因| 广谱是什么意思| 晚上吃芒果有什么好处和坏处| 女人吃藕有什么好处| 裸官是什么意思| 血淀粉酶是检查什么的| 白羊座后面是什么星座| 日晡是什么意思| 大学记过处分有什么影响| 梦到掉牙齿是什么意思| 流涎是什么意思| 脸发红发烫是什么原因| 木瓜是什么季节的| 婴儿眉毛上黄痂是什么| 肾病钾高吃什么食物好| 世界之大无奇不有是什么意思| 耳朵响是什么原因| 灰太狼是什么意思| 公务员做什么工作| 深海鱼油有什么功效| 太息是什么意思| 吃什么食物补肾最快| 心衰挂什么科| 86年属什么的生肖| 男性囊肿是什么原因引起的| 市政协常委是什么级别| 射频消融术是什么意思| 无性恋什么意思| 补钙吃什么维生素| 碱性磷酸酶偏高说明什么问题| 朱顶红什么时候开花| 肺部斑片状高密度影是什么意思| 意什么风发| 不排卵是什么原因| 猜忌是什么意思| 桑葚有什么功效| 10月11号是什么星座| 什么是肩袖损伤| 女人右眼跳是什么预兆| 下午茶一般吃什么| 肝血管瘤有什么症状表现| 吃什么去湿气最好最快| 加尿素起什么作用| 乙肝e抗体阳性是什么意思| 乙肝挂什么科| 胆囊炎不能吃什么食物| 开尔文是什么单位| 巨蟹座有什么特点| 清静是什么意思| 十二指肠球炎是什么病| 天的反义词是什么| 牒是什么意思| 白灼虾是什么虾| 什么什么大名| 中医把脉能看出什么| 癞蛤蟆长什么样| 鼻息肉长什么样| edenbo是什么牌子| 女性黄体期是什么时候| 爆竹声中一岁除下一句是什么| 手麻吃什么药最好| 屁股抽筋疼是什么原因| 百度Jump to content

西安发布限购新政:家庭首套商贷首付比例30%

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
百度 昨天(21日),脸书公司首席执行官扎克伯格首次就此事件公开发声,承认公司犯了错误。

Pre-test probability and post-test probability (alternatively spelled pretest and posttest probability) are the probabilities of the presence of a condition (such as a disease) before and after a diagnostic test, respectively. Post-test probability, in turn, can be positive or negative, depending on whether the test falls out as a positive test or a negative test, respectively. In some cases, it is used for the probability of developing the condition of interest in the future.

Test, in this sense, can refer to any medical test (but usually in the sense of diagnostic tests), and in a broad sense also including questions and even assumptions (such as assuming that the target individual is a female or male). The ability to make a difference between pre- and post-test probabilities of various conditions is a major factor in the indication of medical tests.

Pre-test probability

[edit]

The pre-test probability of an individual can be chosen as one of the following:

  • The prevalence of the disease, which may have to be chosen if no other characteristic is known for the individual, or it can be chosen for ease of calculation even if other characteristics are known although such omission may cause inaccurate results
  • The post-test probability of the condition resulting from one or more preceding tests
  • A rough estimation, which may have to be chosen if more systematic approaches are not possible or efficient

Estimation of post-test probability

[edit]

In clinical practice, post-test probabilities are often just estimated or even guessed. This is usually acceptable in the finding of a pathognomonic sign or symptom, in which case it is almost certain that the target condition is present; or in the absence of finding a sine qua non sign or symptom, in which case it is almost certain that the target condition is absent.

In reality, however, the subjective probability of the presence of a condition is never exactly 0 or 100%. Yet, there are several systematic methods to estimate that probability. Such methods are usually based on previously having performed the test on a reference group in which the presence or absence on the condition is known (or at least estimated by another test that is considered highly accurate, such as by "Gold standard"), in order to establish data of test performance. These data are subsequently used to interpret the test result of any individual tested by the method. An alternative or complement to reference group-based methods is comparing a test result to a previous test on the same individual, which is more common in tests for monitoring.

The most important systematic reference group-based methods to estimate post-test probability includes the ones summarized and compared in the following table, and further described in individual sections below.

Method Establishment of performance data Method of individual interpretation Ability to accurately interpret subsequent tests Additional advantages
By predictive values Direct quotients from reference group Most straightforward: Predictive value equals probability Usually low: Separate reference group required for every subsequent pre-test state Available both for binary and continuous values
By likelihood ratio Derived from sensitivity and specificity Post-test odds given by multiplying pretest odds with the ratio Theoretically limitless Pre-test state (and thus the pre-test probability) does not have to be same as in reference group
By relative risk Quotient of risk among exposed and risk among unexposed Pre-test probability multiplied by the relative risk Low, unless subsequent relative risks are derived from same multivariate regression analysis Relatively intuitive to use
By diagnostic criteria and clinical prediction rules Variable, but usually most tedious Variable Usually excellent for all test included in criteria Usually most preferable if available

By predictive values

[edit]

Predictive values can be used to estimate the post-test probability of an individual if the pre-test probability of the individual can be assumed roughly equal to the prevalence in a reference group on which both test results and knowledge on the presence or absence of the condition (for example a disease, such as may determined by "Gold standard") are available.

If the test result is of a binary classification into either positive or negative tests, then the following table can be made:

Condition
(as determined by "Gold standard")
Positive Negative
Test
outcome
Positive True Positive False Positive
(Type I error)
Positive predictive value
Negative False Negative
(Type II error)
True Negative Negative predictive value

Sensitivity

Specificity

Accuracy

Pre-test probability can be calculated from the diagram as follows:

Pretest probability = (True positive + False negative) / Total sample

Also, in this case, the positive post-test probability (the probability of having the target condition if the test falls out positive), is numerically equal to the positive predictive value, and the negative post-test probability (the probability of having the target condition if the test falls out negative) is numerically complementary to the negative predictive value ([negative post-test probability] = 1 - [negative predictive value]),[1] again assuming that the individual being tested does not have any other risk factors that result in that individual having a different pre-test probability than the reference group used to establish the positive and negative predictive values of the test.

In the diagram above, this positive post-test probability, that is, the posttest probability of a target condition given a positive test result, is calculated as:

Positive posttest probability = True positives / (True positives + False positives)

Similarly:

The post-test probability of disease given a negative result is calculated as:

Negative posttest probability = 1 - (True negatives / (False negatives + True negatives))

The validity of the equations above also depend on that the sample from the population does not have substantial sampling bias that make the groups of those who have the condition and those who do not substantially disproportionate from corresponding prevalence and "non-prevalence" in the population. In effect, the equations above are not valid with merely a case-control study that separately collects one group with the condition and one group without it.

By likelihood ratio

[edit]

The above methods are inappropriate to use if the pretest probability differs from the prevalence in the reference group used to establish, among others, the positive predictive value of the test. Such difference can occur if another test preceded, or the person involved in the diagnostics considers that another pretest probability must be used because of knowledge of, for example, specific complaints, other elements of a medical history, signs in a physical examination, either by calculating on each finding as a test in itself with its own sensitivity and specificity, or at least making a rough estimation of the individual pre-test probability.

In these cases, the prevalence in the reference group is not completely accurate in representing the pre-test probability of the individual, and, consequently, the predictive value (whether positive or negative) is not completely accurate in representing the post-test probability of the individual of having the target condition.

In these cases, a posttest probability can be estimated more accurately by using a likelihood ratio for the test. Likelihood ratio is calculated from sensitivity and specificity of the test, and thereby it does not depend on prevalence in the reference group,[2] and, likewise, it does not change with changed pre-test probability, in contrast to positive or negative predictive values (which would change). Also, in effect, the validity of post-test probability determined from likelihood ratio is not vulnerable to sampling bias in regard to those with and without the condition in the population sample, and can be done as a case-control study that separately gathers those with and without the condition.

Estimation of post-test probability from pre-test probability and likelihood ratio goes as follows:[2]

  • Pretest odds = Pretest probability / (1 - Pretest probability)
  • Posttest odds = Pretest odds * Likelihood ratio

In equation above, positive post-test probability is calculated using the likelihood ratio positive, and the negative post-test probability is calculated using the likelihood ratio negative.

  • Posttest probability = Posttest odds / (Posttest odds + 1)
Fagan nomogram[3]

The relation can also be estimated by a so-called Fagan nomogram (shown at right) by making a straight line from the point of the given pre-test probability to the given likelihood ratio in their scales, which, in turn, estimates the post-test probability at the point where that straight line crosses its scale.

The post-test probability can, in turn, be used as pre-test probability for additional tests if it continues to be calculated in the same manner.[2]

It is possible to do a calculation of likelihood ratios for tests with continuous values or more than two outcomes which is similar to the calculation for dichotomous outcomes. For this purpose, a separate likelihood ratio is calculated for every level of test result and is called interval or stratum specific likelihood ratios.[4]

Example

[edit]

An individual was screened with the test of fecal occult blood (FOB) to estimate the probability for that person having the target condition of bowel cancer, and it fell out positive (blood were detected in stool). Before the test, that individual had a pre-test probability of having bowel cancer of, for example, 3% (0.03), as could have been estimated by evaluation of, for example, the medical history, examination and previous tests of that individual.

The sensitivity, specificity etc. of the FOB test were established with a population sample of 203 people (without such heredity), and fell out as follows:

Patients with bowel cancer
(as confirmed on endoscopy)
Positive Negative
Fecal
occult
blood
screen
test
outcome
Positive TP = 2 FP = 18 → Positive predictive value
= TP / (TP + FP)
= 2 / (2 + 18)
= 2 / 20
= 10%
Negative FN = 1 TN = 182 → Negative predictive value
= TN / (FN + TN)
= 182 / (1 + 182)
= 182 / 183
≈ 99.5%

Sensitivity
= TP / (TP + FN)
= 2 / (2 + 1)
= 2 / 3
≈ 66.67%

Specificity
= TN / (FP + TN)
= 182 / (18 + 182)
= 182 / 200
= 91%

Accuracy
= (TP + TN) / Total
= (2 + 182) / 203
= 184 / 203
= 90.64%

From this, the likelihood ratios of the test can be established:[2]

  1. Likelihood ratio positive = sensitivity / (1 ? specificity) = 66.67% / (1 ? 91%) = 7.4
  2. Likelihood ratio negative = (1 ? sensitivity) / specificity = (1 ? 66.67%) / 91% = 0.37
  • Pretest probability (in this example) = 0.03
  • Pretest odds = 0.03 / (1 - 0.03) = 0.0309
  • Positive posttest odds = 0.0309 * 7.4 = 0.229
  • Positive posttest probability = 0.229 / (0.229 + 1) = 0.186 or 18.6%

Thus, that individual has a post-test probability (or "post-test risk") of 18.6% of having bowel cancer.

The prevalence in the population sample is calculated to be:

  • Prevalence = (2 + 1) / 203 = 0.0148 or 1.48%

The individual's pre-test probability was more than twice that of the population sample, although the individual's post-test probability was less than twice that of the population sample (which is estimated by the positive predictive value of the test of 10%), opposite to what would result by a less accurate method of simply multiplying relative risks.

Specific sources of inaccuracy

[edit]

Specific sources of inaccuracy when using likelihood ratio to determine a post-test probability include interference with determinants or previous tests or overlap of test targets, as explained below:

Interference with test
[edit]

Post-test probability, as estimated from the pre-test probability with likelihood ratio, should be handled with caution in individuals with other determinants (such as risk factors) than the general population, as well as in individuals that have undergone previous tests, because such determinants or tests may also influence the test itself in unpredictive ways, still causing inaccurate results. An example with the risk factor of obesity is that additional abdominal fat can make it difficult to palpate abdominal organs and decrease the resolution of abdominal ultrasonography, and similarly, remnant barium contrast from a previous radiography can interfere with subsequent abdominal examinations,[5] in effect decreasing the sensitivities and specificities of such subsequent tests. On the other hand, the effect of interference can potentially improve the efficacy of subsequent tests as compared to usage in the reference group, such as some abdominal examinations being easier when performed on underweight people.

Overlap of tests
[edit]

Furthermore, the validity of calculations upon any pre-test probability that itself is derived from a previous test depend on that the two tests do not significantly overlap in regard to the target parameter being tested, such as blood tests of substances belonging to one and the same deranged metabolic pathway. An example of the extreme of such an overlap is where the sensitivity and specificity has been established for a blood test detecting "substance X", and likewise for one detecting "substance Y". If, in fact, "substance X" and "substance Y" are one and the same substance, then, making a two consecutive tests of one and the same substance may not have any diagnostic value at all, although the calculation appears to show a difference. In contrast to interference as described above, increasing overlap of tests only decreases their efficacy. In the medical setting, diagnostic validity is increased by combining tests of different modalities to avoid substantial overlap, for example in making a combination of a blood test, a biopsy and radiograph.

Methods to overcome inaccuracy
[edit]

To avoid such sources of inaccuracy by using likelihood ratios, the optimal method would be to gather a large reference group of equivalent individuals, in order to establish separate predictive values for use of the test in such individuals. However, with more knowledge of an individual's medical history, physical examination and previous test etc. that individual becomes more differentiated, with increasing difficulty to find a reference group to establish tailored predictive values, making an estimation of post-test probability by predictive values invalid.

Another method to overcome such inaccuracies is by evaluating the test result in the context of diagnostic criteria, as described in the next section.

By relative risk

[edit]

Post-test probability can sometimes be estimated by multiplying the pre-test probability with a relative risk given by the test. In clinical practice, this is usually applied in evaluation of a medical history of an individual, where the "test" usually is a question (or even assumption) regarding various risk factors, for example, sex, tobacco smoking or weight, but it can potentially be a substantial test such as putting the individual on a weighing scale. When using relative risks, the resultant probability is usually rather related to the individual developing the condition over a period of time (similarly to the incidence in a population), instead of being the probability of an individual of having the condition in the present, but can indirectly be an estimation of the latter.

Usage of hazard ratio can be used somewhat similarly to relative risk.

One risk factor

[edit]

To establish a relative risk, the risk in an exposed group is divided by the risk in an unexposed group.

If only one risk factor of an individual is taken into account, the post-test probability can be estimated by multiplying the relative risk with the risk in the control group. The control group usually represents the unexposed population, but if a very low fraction of the population is exposed, then the prevalence in the general population can often be assumed equal to the prevalence in the control group. In such cases, the post-test probability can be estimated by multiplying the relative risk with the risk in the general population.

For example, the incidence of breast cancer in a woman in the United Kingdom at age 55 to 59 is estimated at 280 cases per 100.000 per year,[6] and the risk factor of having been exposed to high-dose ionizing radiation to the chest (for example, as treatments for other cancers) confers a relative risk of breast cancer between 2.1 and 4.0,[7] compared to unexposed. Because a low fraction of the population is exposed, the prevalence in the unexposed population can be assumed equal to the prevalence in the general population. Subsequently, it can be estimated that a woman in the United Kingdom that is aged between 55 and 59 and that has been exposed to high-dose ionizing radiation should have a risk of developing breast cancer over a period of one year of between 588 and 1.120 in 100.000 (that is, between 0,6% and 1.1%).

Multiple risk factors

[edit]

Theoretically, the total risk in the presence of multiple risk factors can be estimated by multiplying with each relative risk, but is generally much less accurate than using likelihood ratios, and is usually done only because it is much easier to perform when only relative risks are given, compared to, for example, converting the source data to sensitivities and specificities and calculate by likelihood ratios. Likewise, relative risks are often given instead of likelihood ratios in the literature because the former is more intuitive. Sources of inaccuracy of multiplying relative risks include:

  • Relative risks are affected by the prevalence of the condition in the reference group (in contrast to likelihood ratios, which are not), and this issue results in that the validity of post-test probabilities become less valid with increasing difference between the prevalence in the reference group and the pre-test probability for any individual. Any known risk factor or previous test of an individual almost always confers such a difference, decreasing the validity of using relative risks in estimating the total effect of multiple risk factors or tests. Most physicians do not appropriately take such differences in prevalence into account when interpreting test results, which may cause unnecessary testing and diagnostic errors.[8]
  • A separate source of inaccuracy of multiplying several relative risks, considering only positive tests, is that it tends to overestimate the total risk as compared to using likelihood ratios. This overestimation can be explained by the inability of the method to compensate for the fact that the total risk cannot be more than 100%. This overestimation is rather small for small risks, but becomes higher for higher values. For example, the risk of developing breast cancer at an age younger than 40 years in women in the United Kingdom can be estimated at 2%.[9] Also, studies on Ashkenazi Jews has indicated that a mutation in BRCA1 confers a relative risk of 21.6 of developing breast cancer in women under 40 years of age, and a mutation in BRCA2 confers a relative risk of 3.3 of developing breast cancer in women under 40 years of age.[10] From these data, it may be estimated that a woman with a BRCA1 mutation would have a risk of approximately 40% of developing breast cancer at an age younger than 40 years, and woman with a BRCA2 mutation would have a risk of approximately 6%. However, in the rather improbable situation of having both a BRCA1 and a BRCA2 mutation, simply multiplying with both relative risks would result in a risk of over 140% of developing breast cancer before 40 years of age, which can not possibly be accurate in reality.

The (latter mentioned) effect of overestimation can be compensated for by converting risks to odds, and relative risks to odds ratios. However, this does not compensate for (former mentioned) effect of any difference between pre-test probability of an individual and the prevalence in the reference group.

A method to compensate for both sources of inaccuracy above is to establish the relative risks by multivariate regression analysis. However, to retain its validity, relative risks established as such must be multiplied with all the other risk factors in the same regression analysis, and without any addition of other factors beyond the regression analysis.

In addition, multiplying multiple relative risks has the same risk of missing important overlaps of the included risk factors, similarly to when using likelihood ratios. Also, different risk factors can act in synergy, with the result that, for example, two factors that both individually have a relative risk of 2 have a total relative risk of 6 when both are present, or can inhibit each other, somewhat similarly to the interference described for using likelihood ratios.

By diagnostic criteria and clinical prediction rules

[edit]

Most major diseases have established diagnostic criteria and/or clinical prediction rules. The establishment of diagnostic criteria or clinical prediction rules consists of a comprehensive evaluation of many tests that are considered important in estimating the probability of a condition of interest, sometimes also including how to divide it into subgroups, and when and how to treat the condition. Such establishment can include usage of predictive values, likelihood ratios as well as relative risks.

For example, the ACR criteria for systemic lupus erythematosus defines the diagnosis as presence of at least 4 out of 11 findings, each of which can be regarded as a target value of a test with its own sensitivity and specificity. In this case, there has been evaluation of the tests for these target parameters when used in combination in regard to, for example, interference between them and overlap of target parameters, thereby striving to avoid inaccuracies that could otherwise arise if attempting to calculate the probability of the disease using likelihood ratios of the individual tests. Therefore, if diagnostic criteria have been established for a condition, it is generally most appropriate to interpret any post-test probability for that condition in the context of these criteria.

Also, there are risk assessment tools for estimating the combined risk of several risk factors, such as the online tool [1] from the Framingham Heart Study for estimating the risk for coronary heart disease outcomes using multiple risk factors, including age, gender, blood lipids, blood pressure and smoking, being much more accurate than multiplying the individual relative risks of each risk factor.

Still, an experienced physician may estimate the post-test probability (and the actions it motivates) by a broad consideration including criteria and rules in addition to other methods described previously, including both individual risk factors and the performances of tests that have been carried out.

Clinical use of pre- and post-test probabilities

[edit]

A clinically useful parameter is the absolute (rather than relative, and not negative) difference between pre- and post-test probability, calculated as:

Absolute difference = | (pre-test probability) - (post-test probability) |

A major factor for such an absolute difference is the power of the test itself, such as can be described in terms of, for example, sensitivity and specificity or likelihood ratio. Another factor is the pre-test probability, with a lower pre-test probability resulting in a lower absolute difference, with the consequence that even very powerful tests achieve a low absolute difference for very unlikely conditions in an individual (such as rare diseases in the absence of any other indicating sign), but on the other hand, that even tests with low power can make a great difference for highly suspected conditions.

The probabilities in this sense may also need to be considered in context of conditions that are not primary targets of the test, such as profile-relative probabilities in a differential diagnostic procedure.

The absolute difference can be put in relation to the benefit for an individual that a medical test achieves, such as can roughly be estimated as:

, where:

  • bn is the net benefit of performing a medical test
  • Λp is the absolute difference between pre- and posttest probability of conditions (such as diseases) that the test is expected to achieve.
  • ri is the rate of how much probability differences are expected to result in changes in interventions (such as a change from "no treatment" to "administration of low-dose medical treatment").
  • bi is the benefit of changes in interventions for the individual
  • hi is the harm of changes in interventions for the individual, such as side effects of medical treatment
  • ht is the harm caused by the test itself

In this formula, what constitutes benefit or harm largely varies by personal and cultural values, but general conclusions can still be drawn. For example, if the only expected effect of a medical test is to make one disease more likely than another, but the two diseases have the same treatment (or neither can be treated), then ri = 0 and the test is essentially without any benefit for the individual.

Additional factors that influence a decision whether a medical test should be performed or not include: cost of the test, availability of additional tests, potential interference with subsequent test (such as an abdominal palpation potentially inducing intestinal activity whose sounds interfere with a subsequent abdominal auscultation), time taken for the test or other practical or administrative aspects. Also, even if not beneficial for the individual being tested, the results may be useful for the establishment of statistics in order to improve health care for other individuals.

Subjectivity

[edit]

Pre- and post-test probabilities are subjective based on the fact that, in reality, an individual either has the condition or not (with the probability always being 100%), so pre- and post-test probabilities for individuals can rather be regarded as psychological phenomena in the minds of those involved in the diagnostics at hand.

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ Evidence-Based Practice Online Course By Mark Ebell. College of Public Health, University of Georgia. Retrieved Aug 2011
  2. ^ a b c d Likelihood Ratios Archived 22 December 2010 at the Wayback Machine, from CEBM (Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine). Page last edited: 1 February 2009. When used in examples, the general formulas are taken from reference, while example numbers are different
  3. ^ Parameters taken from image in: Zhang W, Doherty M, Pascual E, et al. (October 2006). "EULAR evidence based recommendations for gout. Part I: Diagnosis. Report of a task force of the Standing Committee for International Clinical Studies Including Therapeutics (ESCISIT)". Ann. Rheum. Dis. 65 (10): 1301–11. doi:10.1136/ard.2006.055251. PMC 1798330. PMID 16707533.
  4. ^ Brown MD, Reeves MJ (2003). "Evidence-based emergency medicine/skills for evidence-based emergency care. Interval likelihood ratios: another advantage for the evidence-based diagnostician". Ann Emerg Med. 42 (2): 292–297. doi:10.1067/mem.2003.274. PMID 12883521.
  5. ^ Page 750 (Chapter 10) in: Dunning, Marshall Barnett; Fischbach, Frances Talaska (2009). A manual of laboratory and diagnostic tests [electronic resource]. Philadelphia: Wolters Kluwer Health/Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. ISBN 978-0-7817-7194-8.
  6. ^ Excel chart for Figure 1.1: Breast Cancer (C50), Average Number of New Cases per Year and Age-Specific Incidence Rates, UK, 2006-2008 at Breast cancer – UK incidence statistics Archived 14 May 2012 at the Wayback Machine at Cancer Research UK. Section updated 18 July 2011.
  7. ^ ACS (2005). "Breast Cancer Facts & Figures 2005–2006" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on 13 June 2007. Retrieved 26 April 2007.
  8. ^ Agoritsas, T.; Courvoisier, D. S.; Combescure, C.; Deom, M.; Perneger, T. V. (2010). "Does Prevalence Matter to Physicians in Estimating Post-test Probability of Disease? A Randomized Trial". Journal of General Internal Medicine. 26 (4): 373–378. doi:10.1007/s11606-010-1540-5. PMC 3055966. PMID 21053091.
  9. ^ 2% given from a cumulative incidence 2.075 cases per 100.000 in females younger up to age 39, from the Cancer Research UK reference above.
  10. ^ Satagopan, J. M.; Offit, K.; Foulkes, W.; Robson, M. E.; Wacholder, S.; Eng, C. M.; Karp, S. E.; Begg, C. B. (2001). "The lifetime risks of breast cancer in Ashkenazi Jewish carriers of BRCA1 and BRCA2 mutations". Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention. 10 (5): 467–473. PMID 11352856.
嘴唇红是什么原因 敌敌畏中毒用什么洗胃 gi是什么意思 胃溃疡十二指肠溃疡吃什么药 手比脸白是什么原因
男生为什么喜欢女生 无能为力是什么意思 来龙去脉是什么意思 dym是什么意思 送老师送什么礼物好
蓝莓什么季节成熟 甘露醇是什么药 藕色是什么颜色 澳大利亚属于什么气候 吃什么排毒
622188开头是什么银行 咳嗽吐黄痰是什么原因 曹操原名叫什么 孑然一身是什么意思 十月五号是什么星座
泰山石敢当是什么意思hkuteam.com 萎缩性胃炎吃什么水果好hcv9jop4ns6r.cn 子宫肌瘤有什么危害hcv7jop9ns1r.cn 枣子什么季节成熟hcv7jop9ns4r.cn 强烈的什么hcv9jop0ns3r.cn
黄牛用的什么抢票软件hcv9jop0ns5r.cn 息肉样病变是什么意思hcv7jop5ns3r.cn 骨头受伤了吃什么恢复的快wuhaiwuya.com 家里为什么有蟑螂hcv9jop0ns1r.cn 肾病挂什么科hcv8jop3ns1r.cn
猫藓用什么药xinjiangjialails.com 工字五行属什么clwhiglsz.com 孕妇血糖高对胎儿有什么影响hcv8jop1ns5r.cn 彬字五行属什么hcv8jop7ns5r.cn 白天梦见蛇是什么预兆hcv8jop3ns7r.cn
感冒挂什么科hcv9jop4ns6r.cn 晕菜是什么意思hcv7jop9ns7r.cn 元宵节的习俗是什么hcv8jop3ns7r.cn 七月份能种什么菜hcv8jop0ns8r.cn 梦想成真是什么意思hcv7jop6ns0r.cn
百度